POL Scientific / Bladder / Volume 3 / Issue 2 / DOI: 10.14440/bladder.2016.82
Cite this article
2
Download
131
Citations
525
Views
Journal Browser
Volume | Year
Issue
Search
News and Announcements
View All
RESEARCH ARTICLE

Sensation in the urethra during voiding studied in young healthy men

Jean-Jacques Wyndaele Sigrid Van de Borne Michel Wyndaele Stefan De Wachter
Show Less
1 Urology, UZA, 10 Wilrijkstraat, 2650 Edegem, Belgium
Bladder 2016 , 3(2), 1–7; https://doi.org/10.14440/bladder.2016.82
Published: 9 May 2016
© 2016 by the Author(s). This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License ( https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ )
Abstract

Objectives: Evaluate sensation in the urethra during voiding and the possible role of visual and audible impulses, in young healthy men.

Methods: In 10 healthy volunteers, 2 non-invasive uroflowmetries were done at desire to void and sensations felt during voiding were evaluated by questionnaire. Influence of  visual and audible input was studied by performing a uroflowmetry  while wearing blindfold eye patches and ear cups + earplugs.

Results: All  participants reported sensation of start of voiding, all but one of voiding stop. All felt clearly urine passage in the urethra  and 8 had a sensation of warmth in the urethra. Visual and audible blocking gave lowering in grades of sensation. To start voiding took a significantly longer time. It became difficult for some men  to determine accurately when the flow stopped.

Conclusions: Voiding elicited different types of sensation in the urethra in healthy young men. The sensation was independent of the uroflowmetry parameters. Blocking visual and audible signals influenced the sensory and voiding process.

Keywords
urethra
sensation
voiding
healthy
men

Introduction

Little has been published on the sensation felt in the urethra during voiding, and most studies are from several decades ago.
We studied the urethral voiding sensation in a series of young healthy men, recruited in the community. We also evaluated if visual and audible impulses can have an influence on the sensations during the voiding process.

Materials and Methods

After approval by the local ethics committee, 10 healthy male volunteers where recruited by advertisement. If urological and neurologic history was negative, and a control uroflowmetry at arrival was reported by the participants as being “normal, as in daily life”, a residual was measured with ultrasonography. Participants read the study protocol and signed an informed consent.
Non invasive uroflowmetry was done 2 times on a Type AQTT uroflowmeter (Laborie Medical Technologies Europe) [1]. Normal desire to void was required to start both tests.
A first test uroflowmetry (UROFLOW 1) was done with visual and audible observation possible. For the second test uroflowmetry (UROFLOW 2) participants wore a blindfold eye patch (3M, Opticlude orthopedic eye patch for occlusion therapy) and earcups plus earplugs (Adulte Amplivox auralgard II), blocking visual and audible signals completely. Between tests free drinking was made available (water or soft drinks). All uroflowmetries were done in the sitting position to avoid concern to miss the flowmeter when blinded.
Reporting start and stop of outflow was noted on the curve. Sensation was evaluated after UROFLOW 1 and 2 with scoring “very clearly, clearly, moderate, uncertain/negative” of following statements: “ I could feel the start of the urine flow. I could feel the stop of the urine flow. I could feel urine passing through my urine canal. The passage of the urine stream felt warm”. It was also asked if another sensation had been felt and if affirmative to describe it.
SPSS 22 was used for statistical analysis. Answers are given with the number of participants. Descriptive data are given as median (min-max). P-values are based on Wilcoxon signed rank test in ordinal and scale variables, Mc Nemar test for yes/no questions. Correlations were done with Spearman rho correlation test.

Results

The volunteers had age 27,5 (23 to 34) years; weight 72 (65 to 87) kg; length 182 (169 to 187) cm. All reported the uroflowmetry at arrival as normal for their daily life. Postvoid residual was absent in 8 while two had residue of 9 ml and 10 ml.
The results of the uroflowmetries are given in Table 1. The sensations reported are given in Table 2.
The uroflowmetry curve was of bell shape (7/10 in UROFLOW 1 and 5/10 in UROFLOW 2 ) or shape with important peaks (3/10 in UROFLOW 1 and 5/10 in UROFLOW 2) [1]. In 2 participants a change from bell shape to shape with peaks was seen between UROFLOW 1 and 2. However this last pattern was also noted on the uroflow at arrival and thus was considered as a normal variation.
Reporting stop of the urine outflow while the stream was still running was done by 1 on UROFLOW 1 and by 4 on UROFLOW 2. Two reported stop of the outflow after it had stopped already in UROFLOW 1. In UROFLOW 2 one participant was uncertain if voiding had started when urine was passing.
Three participants reported other sensations during uroflowmetry. Participant N° 6 reported a sensation as of slow building up of the stream (UROFLOW 1) and somewhat lowered stream (UROFLOW 2) though his flow parameters were not different from those of the whole group. Participant N° 8 reported a very relaxed feeling (UROFLOW 1) and tingling calves (UROFLOW 2). Participant N° 9 reported a “not normal sensation as in daily life” during the first test, while no such sensation was reported after UROFLOW 2.
There was no significant correlation between scoring of the statements and uroflowmetry parameters, between time before outflow started and the volume voided, and between the data of UROFLOW 1 and 2, which permits to exclude in this series a clear influence of different flow parameters on the sensations reported.

Table 1. Data from the uroflowmetries.
 

Discussion

Reporting on sensations elicited by activities happening inside the body is by definition subjective and a result of interpretation of sensory impulses reaching consciousness. What is felt and how it is reported is most probably the result of a very complex process: minute observation of one’s own sensation, ease and correctness of expression, psychological and environmental influences. The special sensations reported by 3 participants can illustrate this. What is felt will also depend on input from non- related organs.
That relationship exists between urethral afferents and micturition or bladder function has been shown before in different ways. Involvement of urethral sensory information in desire to void is suggested by the fact that strong desire to void at full bladder is localized very low in the pelvis and urethra, probably related to an increase of intra-urethral pressure at the sphincter level [2]. Various forms of stimulation of the urethra (as introducing a cystoscope or catheter) can cause a desire to void, indicating that estimating that the bladder is full may depend on bladder and on urethral afferent impulses [3]. Studies in patients with different lesions of the innervation showed that these afferent potentials run in the pudendal nerves, and can create a sensation of passing urine [4], of pain, temperature and touch [5]. Anesthetic blocking of urethral afferents can lead to incomplete bladder emptying and the need to strain in healthy volunteers [6]. Sensations occurring with the introduction of a catheter are usually described as similar to those at the onset of micturition, but after 10 or 15 seconds the sensation fades away and patients are then unaware of the catheter within the posterior urethra. This would imply that receptors which have responded to change of stretch adapt to the increased urethral diameter [7]. As the sensation of passage of urine continues during flow such adaptation of receptors involved in flow sensation seems unlikely. In this study we found no correlation between different flow parameters as voided volume, peak flow and flow time and the level or type of sensation reported. What would happen if flow was done under more extreme circumstances, as after holding urine beyond strong desire to void, remains to be studied.
The knowledge of neurophysiological structures related to sensation in the urethra is more limited than the data on bladder [8]. Histologically in the proximal urethra a transition to a stratified or columnar epithelium accompanied by a lack of urothelial-specific differentiation markers and microvilli on the apical surface has been described [9]. The urethral epithelium is part of a signaling system involving projections of the neuroendocrine cells, interstitial cells and sensory nerve endings. George and Dixon [10] described an extensive plexus of nerve fibers throughout the lamina propria and densest immediately beneath the epithelial lining. These are believed to sub serve a sensory function. Specialized sensory endings as found within striated muscle (i.e. muscle spindles) do not occur in the external urethral sphincter [11]. Flow receptors running through the pudendal nerve were suggested by Todd [12].
Histological studies have also shown that the pathways conducting impulses subserving pain and temperature from the urethra run in the same region as that conducting impulses subserving the sensation of full bladder and pain of the bladder [4]. If the spino-thalamic tract is divided, patients might still feel sensation that micturition is impending, that it is progressing, that it has ceased. These sensations would therefore be due to impulses arising in the urethral mucosa, in the urethral sphincter and the perineal muscles that act synergically with the sphincter. Impulses from these structures travel via the posterior columns [5].
In standardized conditions as in our pilot study, healthy young participants declare feeling passage of urine either very clearly or clearly. Nathan [5] proposed 3 components in sensation that urine is passing: an awareness of the opening of the sphincter musculature, a feeling of something passing along the urethra and a thermal sensation. Nathan described sensation of urine passage as only a slight sensation, but in focused young men in study conditions as we had, it becomes much more clear. Thermal sensation, as studied by Nathan [13] was reported by 7/10 and 8/10 of our participants during UROFLOW 1 and UROFLOW 2 respectively. If diagnosing a deficiency of urethral sensation can be an indication of unknown neurologic pathology, as was found in bladder filling sensation [14], needs more research.

Table 2. The grading of sensation statements after UROFLOW 1 and 2 (number of participants).
 
That urethral sensation can be influenced by keeping the eyes closed was mentioned by Nathan in one male patient [5]. When visual ánd audible signals were completely blocked many of our participants found that sensation of passage of urine became less clear. We also noted that voiding took a significant longer time to start, and the feeling that flow had started had become less reliable. Sensation of warmth was independent of blocking visual and audible observation. One can hypothesize that when men during voiding look at the stream and hear the running of the urine in the toilet such extra input can influence the overall sensation related to micturition and the estimation when the stream starts and stops. Such influence will vary between subjects, as we have seen in our study.
Although our study group consists of a well confined sample and our investigation technique was standardized, there are limitations to our observations: the series is small, all participants are men. As all uroflowmetries were done in the sitting position to avoid concern for missing the uroflowmeter, we have no information on the urethral sensation of urine flow in standing position. Each evaluation was done on only one uroflowmetry as this seemed sufficient for studying reporting of sensation. It needs to be evaluated if repeated uroflowmetry would give a different result.
Despite the shortcomings we think that our data bring attention to different aspects of the complex process involved in urethral sensation during voiding, as the type and level of sensation, the influence of visual and audible information, the independence of normal flow volume and speed. More observations are needed, in larger groups of participants, of different age, different gender, and with well-defined pathologies to enlarge the clinical picture and better determine the diagnostic value of such sensory evaluation.

Conclusion

All young healthy men participating in this study reported urethral sensation during voiding. The sensation was independent of the uroflowmetry parameters. Blocking visual and audible information had an influence on the sensory function and the voiding, indicating the complexity of sensation and its interpretation.
References
  1. Schäfer W, Abrams P, Liao L, Mattiasson A, Pesce F, et al. (2002) Good urodynamic practices: uroflowmetry, filling cystometry, and pressure-flow studies. Neurourol Urodyn 21: 261-274. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Wyndaele JJ (1998) The normal pattern of perception of bladder filling during cystometry studied in 38 young healthy volunteers. J Urol 160: 479-481. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Tanagho EA, Miller ER, Meyers FH, Corbett RK (1966) Observations on the dynamics of the bladder neck. Br J Urol 38: 72-84. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Nathan PW, Smith MC (1951) The centripetal pathway from the bladder and urethra within the spinal cord. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiat 14: 262-280. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Nathan PW (1956) Sensations associated with micturition. Br J Urol 28: 126-131. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Shafik A, Shafik AA, El-Sibai O, Ahmed I (2003) Role of positive urethrovesical feedback in vesical evacuation. The concept of a second micturition reflex: the urethrovesical reflex. World J Urol 21: 167-170. doi: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00345-003-0340-5. [View Article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Peters HJ (1979) Innervation der Blase. Anatomie und Physiologie der Blase. In: Stöhrer M , editor . Urologie bei Ruckenmark-verletzten. Berlin, Springer. pp 9-12.
  8. Talaat M (1937) Afferent impulses in the nerves supplying the urinary bladder. J Physiol 89: 1-13. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Birder LA, de Wachter S, Gillespie J, Wyndaele JJ (2014) Urethral sensation: basic mechanisms and clinical expressions. Int J Urol 21 Suppl 1: 13-16. doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/iju.12349. [View Article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. George NJR, Dixon JS (1986) Sensory Disorders of the Bladder and Urethra. Berlin: Springer, p 8. [Google Scholar]
  11. Dixon JS, Gosling JA (1983) Histology and fine structure of the muscularis mucosae of the human urinary bladder. J Anat 136: 265-271. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Todd JR (1964) Afferent impulses in the pudendal nerves of the cat. Q J Exp Physiol Cogn Med Sci 49: 258-267. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Nathan PW (1952) Thermal sensations in the bladder. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 15: 150-151. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Wyndaele JJ (1993) Is impaired perception of bladder filling during cystometry a sign of neuropathy. Br J Uro¬l 71: 270-273. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Share
Back to top
Bladder, Electronic ISSN: 2327-2120 Print ISSN: TBA, Published by POL Scientific